Regular issues
Special Issues



eBio
    eBio Home
    Aim and Scope
    Board of Editors
    Staff
    Editorial Workflow
    Publication Charges
    Subscriptions

Other Eagle Hill Journals
    Northeastern Naturalist
    Southeastern Naturalist
    Caribbean Naturalist
    Neotropical Naturalist
    Urban Naturalist
    Prairie Naturalist
    Eastern Paleontologist
    Journal of North American
        Bat Research
    Journal of the North Atlantic

Eagle Hill Institute Home

An Addition to the Toolkit: An Ethogram for Trichoplusia ni

Logan Pearson1, Chloe Meewes2, Sydney Spencer1, Angela Ayala1, Brad Bailey3, Erin E. Barding1, Ryan Shanks1,*, and Margaret Smith1

1Department of Biology, University of North Georgia, 82 College Circle, Dahlonega, GA 30597. 2Biomedical Sciences, University of Alabama, 1720 University Blvd, Birmingham, AL 35294. 3Department of Mathematics, University of North Georgia, 82 College Circle, Dahlonega, GA 30597. *Corresponding author.

eBio, No. 10 (2024)

Abstract
Animal behaviors are a consequence of complex neurobiological mechanisms and environmental interactions and thus can be used to understand these processes. However, we can only study the diversity of animal behaviors in this way if there is a diversity of ethograms across many species. Therefore, we developed an ethogram for Trichoplusia ni, the Cabbage Looper, which is well studied in other areas but for which an ethogram did not previously exist. Additionally, we demonstrate the usefulness of this ethogram by manipulating a single environmental variable, the presence of another T. ni individual, and demonstrate that some aspects of behavior change while others do not. With the addition of another individual, the type and frequency of behaviors is not altered, but the location where organisms spend time and the distance traveled does change. Therefore, this work rigorously describes T. ni behaviors and uses these descriptions to establish a foundation for behavioral studies in this organism and in comparison to others.

pdf iconDownload Full-text pdf

 

 

Site by Bennett Web & Design Co.
eBio L. Pearson et al. 2024 No. 10 1 2024 eBio 10:1–11 An Addition to the Toolkit: An Ethogram for Trichoplusia ni Logan Pearson1, Chloe Meewes2, Sydney Spencer1, Angela Ayala1, Brad Bailey3, Erin E. Barding1, Ryan Shanks1,*, and Margaret Smith1 Abstract - Animal behaviors are a consequence of complex neurobiological mechanisms and environmental interactions and thus can be used to understand these processes. However, we can only study the diversity of animal behaviors in this way if there is a diversity of ethograms across many species. Therefore, we developed an ethogram for Trichoplusia ni, the Cabbage Looper, which is well studied in other areas but for which an ethogram did not previously exist. Additionally, we demonstrate the usefulness of this ethogram by manipulating a single environmental variable, the presence of another T. ni individual, and demonstrate that some aspects of behavior change while others do not. With the addition of another individual, the type and frequency of behaviors is not altered, but the location where organisms spend time and the distance traveled does change. Therefore, this work rigorously describes T. ni behaviors and uses these descriptions to establish a foundation for behavioral studies in this organism and in comparison to others. Introduction Behavior can vary among organisms due to a combination of genetic, environmental, learned, social, cultural, neurological, and physiological factors (Gérard et al. 2022, London 2017, York 2018). The complex interplay between these factors contributes to the diversity of behaviors observed among animals. To begin to tease apart the complexities, model organisms in a lab are useful because they can be used to study individual aspects of behavior in a controlled environment. However, to understand the full spectrum of animal behaviors, we must use a variety of models that reflect the diversity of animal life. For example, insect models can be particularly useful to understand the link between neurological changes and clearly defined behaviors (Chen and Hong 2018, Kravitz and Hernandez 2015, McClellan and Montgomery 2023, Steinbeck et al. 2020). Trichoplusia ni (Hübner) (Cabbage Looper) (Lepidoptera: Noctuidae) has been studied for a variety of reasons, including its role as an agricultural pest and as part of a variety of host-parasite models (Burke and Strand 2014, Gordon and Strand 2009, Kang et al. 1996). Thus it has established research resources, including a commercially available source of material, basic protocols for lab culture, a sequenced genome (Chen et al. 2018), established cell lines (Fu et al. 2018, Maghodia et al. 2020), and well described anatomy and life history (McEwen and Hervey 1960, Shorey et al. 1962). However, currently, there is a paucity of literature describing detailed behavioral analysis of Lepidoptera in a controlled lab setting. To add to the toolkit available for T. ni research, we aim to develop T. ni as a behavioral model. As is typical of lepidopterans, T. ni has 4 developmental stages (egg, larva, pupa and adult), and thus provides unique and defined developmental windows in which one can study behaviors. Also, they also have well-defined nervous systems that could provide a 1Department of Biology, University of North Georgia, 82 College Circle, Dahlonega, GA 30597. 2Biomedical Sciences, University of Alabama, 1720 University Blvd, Birmingham, AL 35294. 3Department of Mathematics, University of North Georgia, 82 College Circle, Dahlonega, GA 30597. *Corresponding author - (Ryan.Shanks@ung.edu) Associate Editor: Holly Boettger-Tong, Wesleyan College eBio L. Pearson et al. 2024 No. 10 2 model that allows mechanistic questions to be answered (Caveney and Donly 2002, Fuchs et al. 2014, Gallant et al. 2003, Liu et al. 2023, Malutan et al. 2002, McLean et al. 2005, Tang et al. 2019). Neuroscience investigations can use behavioral alterations as an endpoint measurement of change. Development of a comprehensive ethogram that captures the intricate behavioral range of model organisms is a key starting point for these types of studies. To our knowledge, T. ni has no comprehensive ethogram established. Therefore, this study establishes a foundational ethogram for T. ni larvae and describes its usefulness in a lab setting with a comparison of these behaviors in single and paired animal paradigms. Materials and Methods Research population The research population was taken from a general population of T. ni kept in constant culture at 25°C with a 16:8 hr light:dark cycle. Adults were fed a 10% sucrose solution, and all larvae were fed ad lib on cornmeal-based artificial diet (Southland Incorporated). Individuals were separated from the general population in the egg stage and reared in pairs. After hatching, the research individuals in the population were staged every other day, and instar was determined based on head capsule width (McEwen and Hervey 1960). Behavior was observed only in larvae on the first day of their fifth instar, and only one of the pair reared together was used regardless of the behavioral paradigm tested. Behavioral arena and recording To document larval behavior, a total of 20 video recordings of a single-animal paradigm (n = 20 individuals) and 20 videos of a paired-animal paradigm (n = 40 individuals) were recorded for 30 min by an HX-WA03 Panasonic camera. The behavioral arena was the bottom of a pipette-tip box with the dimensions 12.5 cm by 10.2 cm (Fig. 1A). The singleanimal paradigm was defined as one individual in the arena (Fig. 1B), and the paired-animal paradigm was defined as two individuals in the arena simultaneously (Fig. 1C). In both paradigms, the arena had no food included. Different larvae were recorded for each video. Recordings were stopped at 30 min because it was empirically determined that no new behaviors were exhibited after 30 min. Dark purple cardstock was placed underneath the arena to prevent glare as the recordings were taken inside the incubator during the light cycle. The camera was suspended above the arena during recording, and the distance between the camera and the arena was approximately 30 cm. The arena floor was sanded to roughen the plastic surface. The walls of the arena were not sanded, and petroleum jelly was applied along the top edges using a sterile cotton swab to prevent the individuals from crawling out of the arena. In addition, the pipette box was cleaned with ethanol between each use to prevent any residual cues from previous trials. Depending on the behavioral paradigm being recorded, the larvae were placed in different areas of the arena (Fig. 1B and C). For the single-animal behavioral paradigm, the larva was placed in the center of the arena at the beginning of the recording (Fig. 1B). For the paired-animal paradigm, each larva was initially placed in opposing quadrants known as “native” for each animal (digitally labeled A and B in Fig. 1C and D). The quadrant of the opposing animal was termed “non-native” for each animal. There were also 2 other quadrants labeled “neutral”, where no organism was initially placed. For both the single-animal and paired-animal paradigm, the quadrant in which the animals spent the most time was referred to as the “primary” quadrant (Fig. 1B and D). eBio L. Pearson et al. 2024 No. 10 3 Ethogram development The examination of behavior took place once the recordings were taken, with independent coders coding each video. Collaboratively, the coders developed a list of behaviors and their associated descriptions for each paradigm. This baseline ethogram was used to independently record the frequency of each behavior exhibited in 5-min bins over 30 min. Extending the time beyond 30 min did not reveal additional behaviors or unique frequencies. Two coders watched videos independently and named, defined, and documented behaviors exhibited by the individual larva (Table 1, Supplemental Data). The coders then compiled their list of behaviors into a master list that was used to code several more videos. To test the quality of the definitions, the list of behaviors was given to additional coders who were not directly involved in the development of the ethogram to independently identify behaviors. All behaviors unable to be accurately coded were redefined in the ethogram. This process was iterated until there was an average intraclass correlation coefficient (ICC) of 0.89 between new coders. Figure 1. Behavioral arena for coding behavior for single (one animal) and paired (two animals) paradigms. (A) Arena dimensions for all behavioral experiments. (B) The single behavioral paradigm started with the larva in the center of the arena. Distance and time were measured for each quadrant with the primary quadrant representing the one with the greatest value for distance and time. (C) The paired behavioral paradigm started with the larvae (A and B) in opposing quadrants. The starting quadrant for A (native) is the non-native quadrant for B and visa-versa. Quadrants where no larva started were considered neutral. (D) Distance and time were measured for native, non-native, and neutral quadrants. The primary quadrant represents the one quadrant with the greatest value for distance and time for each larva individually. Table 1. Trichoplusia ni ethogram Type of behavior Behavior Description Visual aid Locomotion Inchworm Continuous movement whereby the individual’s prolegs and true legs remain suctioned to the surface and slide toward and away from each other while its body arches upward and stretches out. Additionally, any range of that arch is considered inchworm behavior. (See Supplemental File 1, available online at http://eaglehill.us /ebio-036-Shanks-s1.mp4.). Vertical movement Individual latches both its true legs and prolegs onto the petroleum jelly covered the edges of the arena. (See Supplemental File 2, available online at http://eaglehill.us /ebio-036-Shanks-s2.mp4.). Still Individual exhibits no movement; it is still for at least 5s. (See supplemental File 3, available online at http://eaglehill.us /ebio-036-Shanks-s3.mp4.). Elimination Frass Excretion of feces while the individual is in the arena; larval frass is circular and light brown in color. If the individual releases 1 pellet, record that as 1 frass behavior. (See Supplemental File 4, available online at http://eaglehill. us /ebio-036-Shanks-s4.mp4.). Emesis Individual expels fluid from mouth; larval vomit is a green fluid-like substance. (See Supplemental File 5, available online at http://eaglehill.us / ebio-Shanks-s5.mp4.). Non- Locomotion Head movement Individual’s prolegs and true legs remain attached to the surface of the arena while only its head moves (the area from the tip of its head to the area right before its true legs). (See Supplemental File 6, available online at http:// eaglehill.us /ebio-Shanks-s6.mp4.). Upward extension The individual’s prolegs are suctioned to the surface of the arena and it brings its anterior body upward where it holds a still position as seen in the picture. Individual remains still in this position for longer than 1 second. (See Supplemental File 7, available online at http://eaglehill.us /ebio- Shanks-s7.mp4.). Searching Individual’s prolegs suction to the ground while its thorax extends upward and moves from one direction to another (can move L-R or R-L); during this movement, the individual’s true legs are not attached to the surface of the arena. (See Supplemental File 8, available online at http://eaglehill.us / ebio-Shanks-s8.mp4.). Engagement Wall engagement An individual engages with the walls of the arena by latching its true legs onto the petroleum jelly covered surface and maintaining its proleg grip on the bottom surface of the arena. (See Supplemental File 9, available online at http://eaglehill.us /ebio-Shanks-s9.mp4.). Body engagement Individuals engage with its body by keeping prolegs on the surface and bringing head and true legs backwards/forwards to a part of its body. (See Supplemental File 10, available online at http://eaglehill.us /ebio-Shankss10. mp4.). Frass interaction Individuals interact with their frass by nudging their frass with their head. (See Supplemental File 11, available online at http://eaglehill.us /ebio- Shanks-s11.mp4.). Sociability 1-Sided interaction Any part of individual A or B contacts any body part of individual A or B. (See Supplemental File 12, available online at http://eaglehill.us /ebio- Shanks-s12.mp4.). 2-Sided interaction Individual A or B physically contacts the other individual and this elicits a response whereby the other individual uses its head to push A or B away from itself. (See Supplemental File 13, available online at http://eaglehill.us /ebio-Shanks-s13.mp4.). eBio L. Pearson et al. 2024 No. 10 5 Behavioral coding Once the ethogram was established with strong inter-relater reliability, new coders analyzed 30-min recordings of the individual and paired behavioral paradigms. They were provided with the established ethogram, to which they had not been previously exposed. The coders then tallied each time a behavior was displayed according to the terms and definitions they were given. All behaviors were coded in 5-min bins. Time and locomotion To further understand the locomotion patterns of the larvae studied in both paradigms, the arena was divided into quadrants (Fig. 1). The time spent and total distance traveled in each quadrant were measured. The locomotion distance was measured with a ruler by tracing the movement of the individual using a sheet of transparency paper, and the amount of time spent in each quadrant was tracked for each 5-min bin (Fig . 1B and D). Data analysis For tallies of individual behaviors, high-occurrence behaviors were defined as those exhibited 10 or more times in a 30-min recording. Low occurrence behaviors were defined as those exhibited less than 10 times in a 30-min recording. Independent sample t-tests were performed to determine if the frequency of high-occurrence behaviors differed between single- animal and paired-animal paradigms. Similarly, time and distance traveled in different quadrants, and frequency of one-sided vs two-sided interactions were analyzed with t-tests. Duration of time spent in the primary quadrant across time intervals and paired paradigms were analyzed with ANOVA. Statistical analyses were done in R (v. 4.2.2) and SPSS (v. 29.0). In all graphs, error bars represent the standard error ( SE). Results Ethogram To develop this ethogram for T. ni, we collected behavioral data from 20 single-animal videos and 20 paired-animal videos. We only recorded the larvae in the first day of the fifth instar for consistency and visual purposes (Table 1, Supplemental Data). Figure 2. Coded larval behaviors defined in the ethogram measured over 30 minutes for both the single (dark columns) and paired (light columns) behavioral paradigms. (A) High-occurrence behaviors (B) Low-occurrence behaviors. Error bars represent ± standard error (SE). eBio L. Pearson et al. 2024 No. 10 6 Behavior Using the ethogram, coders tallied behaviors exhibited during 30-min recordings. We were able to distinguish the high-occurrence behaviors (Fig. 2A) from low-occurrence behaviors (Fig. 2B). There was no statistical difference in the frequency of high-occurrence behaviors between single-animal and paired-animal paradigms (inchworm: t58 = 0.02, P = 0.98; searching: t58 = 0.98, P = 0.33; wall engagement: t58 = 1.70, P = 0.09). The scarcity of low-occurrence behaviors did not warrant statistical analysis of single-animal and paired animal paradigms (Fig. 2B). Figure 3. Coded larval average time (A and B) and average distance traveled (C and D) in 30 minutes for both the single (dark columns) and paired (light columns) behavioral paradigms. * indicates P < 0.001. Error bars represent ± SE. Figure 4. Average time spent in the primary quadrant for larvae in both the single (dark triangle and solid linear trend line) and paired behavioral paradigms (light triangles and dashed linear trend line) shown in 5-minute time intervals for the 30-minute coding perio d. Error bars represent ± SE. A. B. 0 200 400 600 800 1000 1200 1400 Native quadrant Neutral quadrants Non-native quadrant Average time (s) 0 200 400 600 800 1000 1200 1400 Primary quadrant Other quadrants Average time (s) C. D. 0 50 100 150 200 250 Native quadrant Neutral quadrants Non-native quadrant Average distance (cm) * * * 0 50 100 150 200 250 Single Paired Average distance (cm) eBio L. Pearson et al. 2024 No. 10 7 Time and distance Time and distance traveled in different quadrants was measured for both single-animal and paired-animal paradigms. The single-animal paradigm revealed more time spent in 1 quadrant compared to the other 3 quadrants (Fig. 3A, t38 = 6.8, P < 0.001). The paired-animal paradigm also showed that there was an effect of quadrant on time spent in the quadrant (Fig. 3B, F(2,117) = 11.26, P < 0.001). A post-hoc Tukey’s test indicated that individuals spent less time in the non-native (P < 0.001) quadrant than either the native or neutral quadrants. There was no statistical difference in the total distance traveled by individuals in the single-animal paradigm versus paired-animal paradigm (Fig. 3C, t58 = 0.89, P = 0.37). In contrast, for the paired-animal paradigm, (Fig. 3D), there was a significant difference in the average distance traveled in different quadrants (F(2,117) = 12.23, P < 0.001). A post-hoc Tukey’s test indicated that individuals spent significantly more time in neutral quadrants than either their native quadrant (P < 0.001) or non-native quadrant (P < 0.001). This increase is attributed to the fact there is twice as much space in the neutral quadrants, yet remains notable given the decreased time spent in the non-native quadrant only. Time interval vs duration in primary quadrant The duration of time spent in the primary quadrant across time intervals was also examined (Fig. 4). For the single-animal paradigm, there was no significant difference between the 5-min time intervals and time spent in a primary quadrant versus other quadrants as indicated by a regression line slope not statistically significantly distinguishable from 0 (F(5) = 0.921, P = 0.339). In contrast, for the paired-animal paradigm, individuals decreased the amount of time they spent in their primary quadrant over time as indicated by a statistically significant negative slope of the regression line ( F(5) = 5.81, P = 0.017). Interactions Lastly, 2 novel behaviors (one-sided interactions and two-sided interactions) occurred only in the paired-animal paradigm recordings (Table 1, Fig. 5). One-sided interactions were significantly more frequent than two-sided interactions (t78 = 2.77, P = 0.007). Figure 5. One-sided and twosided interactions measured between larvae in the paired-animal paradigm over 30 minutes. * indicates P < 0.05. Error bars represent ± SE. 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 One-sided interaction Two-sided interaction Average occurrence * eBio L. Pearson et al. 2024 No. 10 8 Discussion Ethogram Establishing a well-defined ethogram is fundamental to understanding animal behavior (Okuyama et al. 2013). With clear operational definitions, this ethogram was created to give other researchers the opportunity to use a straightforward system to answer additional questions (Xu et al. 2012). In addition to these behaviors, methodology for how to track and time the movement of animals within a defined arena provide a template for future studies (Fig. 1, Table 1, Supplemental Data). Whilst many researchers have investigated specific behaviors of interest, a more inclusive description of behaviors allows for a more integrative and cross-disciplinary approach (Collie et al. 2020, McLellan and Montgomery 2023, Okuyama et al. 2013, Peric-Mataruga et al. 2017, Suszczynska et al. 2017). Behavior To illustrate the application of the operational definitions in the ethogram, the behaviors were coded with the addition of a single variable that compares a single larva’s behaviors alone and with an additional larva. High occurrence behaviors show no statistical difference with this single environmental variable we exampled (single vs. paired), yet this may provide an interesting comparison to the effect of different environmental variables such as food availability and temperature (Fig. 2A). Similarly, while low frequency behaviors hold no validity statistically due to the low number of coded events, there were trending differences that may hold biological meaning (Fig. 2B). Therefore, the importance of these recorded events lies beyond any statistical findings. This ethogram and the example data analysis clearly defined all possible behaviors so that future studies may alter environmental, social, or developmental variables or compare behaviors between species in a more wholistic approach. For example, the upward extension of the head provided little measurable difference between coded behaviors within either the single or paired behavioral paradigms, yet this behavior may be drastically altered if food were introduced into a competitive environment in which two animals shared the arena. However, for the ability to conduct future comparative and meta-analysis, it will be important that all behaviors regardless of their statistical significance be documented. Location and distance As with behavior, measures of location and distance traveled provide an important measurement of neurobiobehavioral alterations. This can be used to measure responses to the introduction or removal of environmental cues and gauge whether they are appetitive or non-appetitive. Individuals in both the single and paired behavioral paradigms had unique location and distance patterns of behavior. These patterns of behavior provide a unique baseline data set to investigate alterations in these behaviors including investigations of motivation, competition, and social interactions. For example, the addition of frass or a potential food source in one area of the arena may alter the primary quadrant for an individual in a single behavioral paradigm. In a paired behavioral paradigm these additions may alter the duration that individuals display in a primary quadrant over time (Fig. 4). Interestingly, the presence of an additional organism did not impact the distance traveled by individuals, yet individuals in both the single and paired behavioral paradigms did have unique location and locomotion patterns (Fig. 3). Individuals in the paired behavioral paradigm did not travel longer distances in their native quadrant; however, they spent more time in the native quadrant (Fig. 3). The added individual B in the paired paraeBio L. Pearson et al. 2024 No. 10 9 digm could help explain why individual A spent more time in the native quadrant whilst at the same time traveled less in said quadrant (Fig. 1 and 4). Therefore, it is the combination of both time spent and distance traveled together that is important to consider. Additionally, by binning the duration spent in specific quadrants, unique trends were observed in the individuals of the single and paired behavioral paradigms (Fig. 4). Manipulation of this simple variable (paired vs. single) alters behavior in some measurable and meaningful ways (location, Fig. 3) yet has little effect on other behaviors (high-occurrence, Fig. 2). This highlights the usefulness of establishing an ethogram using a controlled laboratory environment in which an arena of a defined size is used. Interaction Individuals in the paired-animal paradigm exhibited seemingly aggressive one-sided and two-sided interactions. This is not unusual, even to the point of cannibalism, in both eusocial and solitary insects where its function is used to defend territories, establish social hierarchies, and compete for food (Bowen et al. 2008, Collie et al. 2020, Dial and Adler, 1990, Kemp 2000, Semlitsch and West 1988, Tang et al. 2016, Yack et al. 2001, Zago- Braga and Zucoloto 2004, Zhou et al. 2016). While T. ni is not a eusocial insect, it can live in groups, like other lepidopterans, and this may impact behavior (Daly et al. 2012). Figure 5 documents significantly more occurrences of one-sided interactions compared to two-sided interactions in the paired-animal behavioral paradigm. The role these interactions have in the establishment of a potential social hierarchy in T. ni remains unclear, but this foundational data opens the door to investigations of how size, developmental stage, and fitness relate to the establishment of potential dominant and submissive individuals in a population. Furthermore, these investigations relate to more broad questions of how social interactions lead to the evolution of important survival behaviors in a group setting. The combined study of interactions and other behaviors in this ethogram serves as a valuable tool for researchers to further investigate the ecological, physiological, and behavioral aspects of T. ni, aiding in the understanding of its biology and ecology. It could open the door to investigations of more broad behavioral notions such as learned helplessness, usually studied in vertebrate models (Maier and Seligman 2017), or other cued social interactions that usually depend on multiple sensory inputs in insects as well as the underlying neural mechanisms dictating these behaviors (Chen and Hong 2018). The analysis of behavioral data in this ethogram can be used as a reference of a standard social interaction amongst T.ni and instigate the study of the underlying neural mechanisms causing these interactions to occur. Understanding the neural basis of T. ni behavior can shed light on the neural circuits, sensory processing, and neurophysiological processes involved in not only insect but more complex animal behavior. This research provides a foundation for future studies on T. ni and other lepidopteran species, with the potential to uncover novel insights into the neural mechanisms underlying insect behavior and inspire future research in neuroethology. Acknowledgments We thank University of North Georgia’s Department of Biology and Center for Undergraduate Research and Creative Activities for their financial support for this project. We greatly appreciate the animal care and technical assistance provided by Shaswat Patel, Laura-Kate Gleaton, and Maria Fernanda Sanchez Acosta. eBio L. Pearson et al. 2024 No. 10 10 Literature Cited Bowen, J.L., S.J. Mahony, A.C. Mason, and J.E. Yack. 2008. Vibration-mediated territoriality in the warty birch caterpillar Drepana bilineata. Physiological Entomology 33:238–250. Burke, G.R., and M.R. Strand. 2014. Systematic analysis of a wasp parasitism arsenal. Molecular Ecology 23:890–901. Caveney, S., and B.C. Donly. 2002. Neurotransmitter transporters in the insect nervous system. Advances in Insect Physiology 29:55–150. Chen, P., and W. Hong. 2018. Neural circuit mechanisms of social behavior. Neuron 98:16–30. Chen, W., X. Yang, G. Tetreau, X. Song, C. Coutu, D. Hegedus, G. Blissard, Z. Fei, and P. Wang. 2018. A high-quality chromosome-level genome assembly of a generalist herbivore, Trichoplusia ni. Molecular Ecology Resources 19:485–496. Collie, J., O. Granela, E.B. Brown, and A.C. Keene. 2020. Aggression is induced by resource limitation in the monarch caterpillar. iScience 23(12):101791. Daley, D., A.D. Higginson, D. Chen, G.D. Ruxton, and M.P. Speed. 2012. Density-dependent investment in costly anti-predator defenses: An explanation for the weak survival benefit of group living. Ecology Letters 15:576–583. Dial, C.I., and P.H. Adler. 1990. Larval behavior and cannibalism in Heliothis zea (Lepidoptera: Noctuidae). Annals of the Entomological Society of America 83:258–263. Fu, Y., Y. Yang, H. Zhang, G. Farley, J. Wang, K.A. Quarles, Z. Weng, and P.D. Zamore. 2018. The genome of the Hi5 germ cell line from Trichoplusia ni, an agricultural pest and novel model for small RNA biology. eLife 7:e31628. Fuchs, S., E. Rende, A. Crisanti, and T. Nolan. 2014. Disruption of aminergic signalling reveals novel compounds with distinct inhibitory effects on mosquito reproduction, locomotor function and survival. Scientific Reports 4:5526. Gallant, P., T. Malutan, H. McLean, L. Verellen, S. Caveney, and C. Donly. 2003. Functionally distinct dopamine and octopamine transporters in the CNS of the cabbage looper moth. European Journal of Biochemistry 270(4):664–674. Gérard, M., B. Cariou, M. Henrion, C. Descamps, and E. Baird. 2022. Exposure to elevated temperature during development affects bumblebee foraging behavior. Behavioral Ecology 33(4):816– 824. Gordon, S.D., and M.R. Strand. 2009. The polyembryonic wasp Copidosoma floridanum produces two castes by differentially parceling the germ line to daughter embryos during embryo proliferation. Development Genes and Evolution 219:445–454. Kang, D., A. Lundstrom, and H. Steiner. 1996. Trichoplusia ni attacin A, a differentially displayed insect gene coding for an antibacterial protein. Gene 174:245–2 49. Kemp, D.J. 2000. Contest behavior in territorial male butterflies: Does size matter? Behavioral Ecology 11(6):591–596. Kravitz, E.A., and M. Hernandez. 2015. Aggression in Drosophila. Behavioral Neuroscience 129(5):549–563. Liu, X., S. Yang, L. Sun, G. Xie, W. Chen, Y. Liu, G. Wang, X. Yin, and X. Zhao. 2023. Distribution and organization of descending neurons in the brain of adult Helicoverpa armigera (Insecta). Insects 14:63. London, S.E. 2017. Developmental song learning as a model to understand neural mechanisms that limit and promote the ability to learn. Behavioural Processes 1 63:13–23. McLean, H., L. Verellen, S. Caveney, and C. Donly. 2005. Molecular cloning and functional characterization of a neuronal choline transporter from Trichoplusia ni. Insect Biochemistry and Molecular Biology 35:61–72. McClellan, C.F. and S.H. Montgomery. 2023. Towards an integrative approach to understanding collective behavior in caterpillars. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B 378:20220072. McEwen F.L., and G.E.R. Hervey. 1960. Mass-rearing the cabbage looper, Trichoplusia ni, with notes on its biology in the laboratory. Annals of the Entomological Society of America 53(2):229–234. eBio L. Pearson et al. 2024 No. 10 11 Maier, S.F., and M.E.P. Seligman. 2017. Learned helplessness at fifty: Insights from neuroscience. Psychological Reviews 123(4):349–367. Maghodia, A.B., C. Geisler, and D.L. Jarvis. 2020. A new nodavirus-negative Trichoplusia ni cell line for baculovirus-mediated protein production. Biotechnology and Bioengineering 117:3248–3264. Malutan, T., H. McLean, S. Caveney, and C. Donly. 2002. A high-affinity octopamine transporter cloned from the central nervous system of cabbage looper Trichoplusia ni. Insect Biochemistry and Molecular Biology 32:343–357. Okuyama, J., K. Nakajima, T. Noda, S. Kimura, H. Kamihata, M. Kobayashi, and H. Yamada. 2013. Ethogram of immature green turtles: Behavioral strategies for somatic growth in large marine herbivores. PLoS ONE 8(6):e65783. Peric-Mataruga, V., B. Petkovic, L. Ilijin, M. Mrdakovic, S.D. Cucakovic, D. Todorovic, and M. Vlahovic. 2017. Cadmium and high temperature effects on brain and behaviour of Lymantria dispar L. caterpillars originating from polluted and less-polluted for ests. Chemosphere 185:628–636. Semlitsch, R.D., and C.A West. 1988. Size-dependent cannibalism in noctuid caterpillars. Oecologia 77:286–288. Shorey, H.H., L.A. Andres, and R.L. Hale. 1962. The biology of Trichoplusia ni (Lepidoptera: Noctuidae), life history and behavior. Annals of the Entomological Society of America 55(5):591–597. Steinbeck, F., A. Adden, and P. Graham. 2020. Connecting brain to behavior: a role for general purpose steering circuits in insect orientation? Journal of Experi mental Biology 223(5): 212332. Suszczynska, A., M.M. Kaniewska, P. Bebas, J.M. Giebultowicz, and J. Kotwica-Rolinska. 2017. Circadian regulation of caterpillar feeding and growth. Journal of Insect Physiology 101:113–122. Tang, T., C. Zhao, L. Xu, and L. Qiu. 2016. Factors affecting larval cannibalism in the cotton bollworm, Helicoverpa armigera (Hübner) (Lepidoptera: Noctuidae). Oriental Insects 50(1):23–33 . Tang, Q., W. Song, Y. Chang, G. Xie, W. Chen, and X. Zhao. 2019. Distribution of serotonin-immunoreactive neurons in the brain and gnathal ganglion of caterpillar Helicoverpa armigera. Frontiers in Neuroanatomy 13:56. Xu, F., L. Xie, X. Li, Q. Li, T. Wang, Y Ji, F. Kong, Q. Zhan, K. Cheng, L. Fang, and P. Xie. 2012. Construction and validation of a systematic ethogram of Macaca fascicularis in a free enclosure. PLoS ONE 7(5): e37486. Yack, J.E., M.L. Smith, and P.J. Weatherhead. 2001. Caterpillar talk: Acoustically mediated territoriality in larval Lepidoptera. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 98(20):1371–1375. York, R.A. 2018. Assessing the genetic landscape of animal behavior. Genetics 209(1):223–232. Zago-Braga, R.C., and F.S. Zucoloto. 2004. Cannibalism studies on eggs and newly hatched caterpillars in a wild population of Ascia monuste (Godart) (Lepidoptera, Pieridae). Revista Brasileira de Entomologia 48(3):415–420. Zhou, J., M.R. Dudash, and C.B. Fenster. 2016. Cannibalism during early larval development of Hadenaectypa Morrison (Lepidoptera: Noctuidae). Proceedings of the Entomological Society of Washington 118(3):450–455.